Ehrenberg-BassSponsor Website  
    University of South Australia Ehrenberg-Bass Institute for Marketing Science University of South Australia Ehrenberg-Bass Institute for Marketing Science
Log Out
  • Home
  • Online Courses
    • Mining Panel Data for Insights
    • Six Simple Steps of Data Reduction
  • Questions & Feedback
  • Buy Books
  • Additional Services
    • Specialist Research Services
    • How Brands Grow – Live!
    • Other Collaborations
  • Podcast Interviews
Ehrenberg-Bass Institute for Marketing Science

Ehrenberg-BassSponsor Website

Select a category
Search
  • All Categories
  • All Categories
  • # Latest Research
  • Advertising
  • Best Practice
  • Beyond :30
  • Brand Building & Growth
  • Brand Competition
  • ad spend
  • Budgeting
  • Business-to-Business (B2B)
  • Category Entry Points
  • Category Growth
  • Buyer Behaviour
  • Consumer Behaviour
  • Market Research
  • Data Presentation & Method
  • Distinctiveness & Distinctive Assets
  • Double Jeopardy
  • Durables
  • Emerging Markets
  • Innovation
  • Light & Heavy Buyers
  • Loyalty & Defection
  • Loyalty Programs
  • Luxury Brands
  • Marketing Myths
  • Media Decisions
  • Mental Availability & Salience
  • digital
  • Online
  • Packaging Design
  • Pareto Share
  • Penetration and Brand Metrics
  • Physical Availability
  • Portfolio Management
  • Price Promotions & Discounting
  • Pricing Decisions
  • Private Labels
  • qotw
  • Question of the Week
  • Coronavirus
  • Virus
  • Covid
  • COV19
  • COV-19
  • Recessions
  • Segmentation & Targeting
  • Services & Service Quality
  • Shopper Behaviour
  • social marketing
  • Social Cause Marketing
  • Social Media
  • Television
  • Word-of-Mouth

It really is harder to get non-users to remember your advertising

  • Report 87
  • Kelly Vaughan, Virginia Beal and Jenni Romaniuk
  • May 2018

Abstract

A long standing empirical generalisation is that brand users are 2-3 times more likely to notice advertising for the brand they use, than are non-users of the brand, but who are category users. The generalisation was observed in advertising awareness measures where the brand name was either part of the cue or retrieval process. We wondered if the presence of the brand name in the question was creating the user bias artefact. This had us worried that the empirical finding might be an artefact of the measurement approach and using another advertising awareness measure would give us different results.

So we extended our testing to other types of advertising awareness approaches where the brand name is not involved, such as execution-prompted or media-prompted. We find the empirical generalisation still holds, confirming that user bias is a real phenomenon and not an artefact of the measure used.

This means when marketers assess the effectiveness of their brand’s advertising, it is necessary to examine the results for brand users and non-users separately. This is of heightened importance, when comparing advertising awareness metrics for brands of different market shares. This helps provide an apples with apples comparison, across campaigns and brands.

Background

Advertising effectiveness measures provide tools to evaluate a brand’s advertising and give marketers an early indication of advertising performance. Correct measurement is crucial as results often determine future investment for specific executions. Advertising awareness is a common measure used to identify whether there are lasting traces of a brand’s advertising in the memory of category buyers. 

Sharp, Beal and Romaniuk (2001) put forward, as a potential scientific law, the pattern that brand users are more likely than non-users, to say they remember seeing a brand’s advertising. Since then, there has been solid empirical support for the law-like nature of the pattern (Hammer & Riebe 2006; Harrison 2013; Romaniuk & Wight 2009; Sharp et al. 2001, 2002). However, this work testing the law has mostly concentrated on two approaches to questioning:  

  • Which brands of <insert category> have you seen advertising for <insert time period>? 
  • Have you seen any advertising for <insert brand>?

From these questions three advertising awareness metrics can be calculated: 

  • Top-of-mind recall (first brand remembered as advertising); 
  • Unprompted recall (any unprompted memory of the brand advertising); and 
  • Brand prompted recall (memory for the brand advertising with the brand name provided). 

What unites these three metrics is that they involve the brand name, either in the cue or the retrieved information. However, given correct branding for advertising is often reported as much lower than the optimal 100% and advertising exposure can also be remembered for specific creative executions or media channels, we wondered if the presence of the brand name in the question was creating the user bias artefact, such that the empirical generalisation disappears if the advertising awareness question does not involve the brand name. 

Therefore, we set about to specifically investigate whether the bias for brand users to remember advertising more than non-users is a real reflection of differential attention/processing of advertising, or a measurement artefact that can be avoided through using a different advertising awareness measure. 

Our approach

We examine six different advertising awareness measures, each employing a different retrieval cue: 1) top-of-mind recall; 2) unprompted recall; 3) brand prompted; 4) brand plus media prompted; 5) execution prompted; and 6) execution plus media prompted. A brief description of the cueing material for each measure is presented in Table 1.

Table 1 – Summary of awareness measures and examples of questioning techniques

The data

The analysis involved 26 different data sets, covering a wide range of conditions with 101 executions, 88 brands, 18 categories, 10 countries (Australia, China, India, Portugal, Russia, South Africa, Spain, Taiwan, Turkey and UK), with advertisements sourced from a range of different media formats (TV, print, radio, outdoor and online). The samples within each data set included category buyers only.

Results

We find across all six advertising awareness measures tested that brand users are more likely to remember advertising for their brand than are non-users. Overall, 92% of the 247 observations were in favour of brand users remembering advertising, compared to non-users, with an average difference of 18 percentage points. The average results for the six different measurement approaches is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1 – Comparison of advertising awareness levels of brand users versus non-users

Across all six advertising awareness measures, brand users are more likely to remember advertising than non-users of the same brand.

 

We also examined if the different types of cues included in each of the awareness measures explain the difference in scores¹. The results show that using execution-prompted cues, either visual or verbal², reduces the difference between brand user and non-user awareness levels, but prompting for the brand name or media type had no impact. This suggests that the richer cues in an execution-prompted awareness measure makes retrieval of advertising information from memory easier for both brand users and non-users, but non-users will still demonstrate lower levels of retrieval. 

Implications

Our findings suggest that the measures themselves do not enhance or mitigate this user bias. Conversely, each measure simply reflects a bias that is inherently present in buyer memory to a different degree depending on the measure’s difficulty (in terms of its difficulty as a ‘recall from memory’ task). The use of easier measures (i.e. those that provide rich memory cues) heightens response levels from both users and non-users, although a disparity in favour of users remains. In contrast, the use of more difficult measures, with sparse memory cues, will dampen both user and non-user response levels, but non-users to a greater degree.

For marketers this means that the measurement approach chosen to assess the effectiveness of a brand’s advertising should be one that is an easier memory task for all respondents to avoid exacerbating the usage bias.

We further strongly encourage marketers (regardless of the advertising awareness measure used) to always separate out responses for brand users from non-users to account for the usage bias that is ubiquitous across all advertising awareness measures. For smaller brands, this will avoid under-stating awareness levels. As the smaller group of brand users with higher awareness will be washed out at the aggregate level, where the larger proportion of non-users are likely to not remember having seen the advertising. For bigger brands, the reverse is true, with splitting responses ensuring that the impact of advertising is not over-stated due to the larger base of brand users (compared to non-users). This is also beneficial for global brands where the same brand may vary in size in different markets. Separating results will allow better comparison of awareness levels for both brand users and non-users, rather than considering aggregate results across markets, which will be affected by the variation in the size of the user base in each country.

The results of this study, combined with the vast empirical evidence that shows that for a brand to grow it must attract new customers, clearly suggests that a brand’s advertising should be developed with non-brand users in mind. This (important) group of potential customers for a brand likely has very little existing brand knowledge in memory. Therefore, any exposure to advertising has to work harder to first gain attention and build new associations with the brand in memory.  

The next stage of our research is to examine the factors that might lead to greater non-user attention, such as dual branding, to attract the attention of a second brand’s customer base, or holding back on the brand reveal to try to keep non-brand user attention for longer. Initial results say this does not matter, with users and non-users having similar advertising awareness scores in either condition (for more detail see Jenni Romaniuk’s new book on distinctive brand assets). Other research is also examining whether brand users and non-users respond differently to biometric measures. Stay tuned for results on these in the future.


¹ The overall fit of the model R2 = 79%, F =105.4, p<0.001.

² Verbal ß =0.21, p<0.01; Visual ß =0.12, p<0.05.


 

REFERENCE LIST

Hammer, P. & Riebe, E. 2006, ‘Broadening the empirical generalisation: the impact of brand usage on memories of advertising’, Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy (ANZMAC) Conference, Brisbane, Queensland, May 2007.

Harrison, F. 2013, ‘Digging Deeper Down into the Empirical Generalization of Brand Recall’, Journal of Advertising Research, vol. 53, no. 2, pp. 181.

Romaniuk, J. & Wight, S. 2009, ‘The influence of brand usage on responses to advertising awareness measures’, International Journal of Market Research, vol. 51, no. 2, pp. 203.

Sharp, B., Beal, V. & Romaniuk, J. 2001, ‘First steps towards a marketing empirical generalisation: brand usage and subsequent advertising recall’, Collins, SCB (ed), ANZMAC, Auckland, 3-5 December.

Sharp, B., Beal, V. & Romaniuk, J. 2002, ‘Quantifying an Empirical Generalisation: Usage and Advertising Recall in the International Travel Market’, ANZMAC, Melbourne, 2-4 December.

RELATED CATEGORIES

  • Advertising
  • Data Presentation & Method
Content from the Ehrenberg-Bass Institute website for Corporate Sponsors: https://sponsors.marketingscience.info
This content is exclusively for the use of members of the Ehrenberg-Bass Institute Corporate Sponsorship Program.

Can’t find what you are looking for? or have some feedback about the site?                  Contact Us

FOLLOW US

Contact

Phone: +61 8 8302 0111 Postal Address:
GPO Box 2471
Adelaide SA 5001
Australia
Freecall: 1800 801 857 (within Australia) Fax: +61 8 8302 0123 Email: info@MarketingScience.info

Sitemap

  • Home
  • About the Institute
  • Awards and Accolades
  • Ehrenberg-Bass Sponsorship
  • Specialist Research Services
  • News & Media
  • Contact Us
  • Disclaimers, Privacy & Copyright

Corporate Sponsors Member’s Area

  • Sponsor Website Home
  • Online Courses
  • Questions & Feedback
  • Buy Books
  • Research Services

Corporate Sponsors Member’s Area

  • Sponsor Website Home
  • Online Courses
  • Questions & Feedback
  • Buy Books
  • Research Services
image-description

Now available as an eBook exclusively to Apple iBooks

image-description

The Ehrenberg-Bass Institute for Marketing Science is the world’s largest centre for research into marketing. Our team of market research experts can help you grow your brand and develop a culture of evidence-based marketing.

Acknowledgement of Country

Ehrenberg-Bass Institute acknowledges the Traditional Owners of the lands across Australia as the continuing custodians of Country and Culture. We pay our respect to First Nations people and their Elders, past and present.

University of south Australia

The Ehrenberg-Bass Institute is based at the University of South Australia

Website designed & developed by

Website designed & developed by Atomix